I am an interdisciplinary
scientist and engineer with
- a BS in Electrical
Engineering
from University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri.
- an MS in Physics from
Michigan
State University, East Lansing, Michigan.
- and a PhD in
Physiological Optics
from Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana.
After receiving the MS in
Physics,
I worked a year as a teacher and then spent a few years as an engineer
in a large factory. The factory work was fascinating because of the
extent to which they made complex systems from simple materials. Raw
chunks of steel and spools of wire were made into motors and generators
and even into entire locomotives. The factory was also a sort of museum
of bad
lighting. Many of the big factory areas were lit with high pressure
mercury vapor lights. These lamps present a small bright area, which is
good, but they really lose colors. Mercury lights are used as examples
in at least two of the articles linked from the home page: Color
rendering, a new calculation that estimates colorimetric shifts,
and the draft article Vectorial Color
.
A stairway in one
building had low
pressure mercury vapor lights, something you will seldom see. These
lights emit much of their light in the blue end of the spectrum,
probably the 436 nm emission of mercury. Color contrasts are lost,
which is bad, but there is also a special problem with blue light. The
blue receptors in the eye are the least numerous, so vision with the
blue receptors has a dreamy quality, lacking detail. Also, the mercury
arc in these lamps would wander around the bulb, causing shadows to
waver. I mention these experiences to explain how I got interested in
lighting.
I could see that lighting designs exerted strong control on a person's
ability to see, but that the topic was not being discussed in clear
terms.
It was with all this in
mind that
I went to graduate school a second time to study Physiological Optics.
As a graduate degree program, Physiological Optics is the science of
Human Vision as taught in colleges of Optometry. It is considered to be
a broad interdisciplinary field, since vision involves optics and other
physics, experimental psychology, physiology, and even genetics. I
always kept my interest in physics and engineering. The term
Physiological Optics arose in the 19th century when early research on
human vision was done by physicists such as Hermann von Helmholtz.
People say that "you can
never
step in the same river twice," and that is certainly true of my
experiences and education. Today at Indiana University and elsewhere,
Physiological Optics is not taught in the
same way---and there may be greater emphasis on things with a medical
flavor. There may be more specific demands, such as a rule that Ph.D.
students must work with an electron microscope, or work on research
that has a sponsor.
In Color rendering,
a new
calculation that estimates colorimetric shifts, I give specific
credit to one of my physics professors, Peter Signell. He was a tall,
strong person who was able to write fast and clear on the blackboard,
using a stout stick of chalk. Some of the important applied math came
back to me from his lectures, not from any textbook. In the new work on
vectorial color, I was guided by my early instruction on color
vision from Professor Ronald W. Everson. Ron Everson taught the
standard color vision material in a precise way that I clearly recall.
We were left with an open question: "How is it that the chromaticity
diagram identifies the possible mixtures of two given colors,
presenting this information graphically and intuitively, but for the
related question of how much of each light goes into the mixture, no
graphical or intuitive method is given?" In 2003 and 2004, this
question came back to me as I was studying a color space based on
orthonormalized color matching functions. The spectrum locus in this
space conveys the facts of color mixing with the amplitude information
not left out. Ron Everson helped to frame this question, and when the
answer was ready, it presented itself. Technical detail is on another
page, monochromat.html.
Why is that web page
called monochromat.html? In part, that
involves
another personal experience. In the color space of the orthonormal
basis, Jozef Cohen called the spectrum locus "the locus of unit
monochromats." By this he meant that if you could make a narrow-band
light of wavelength lambda and unit power, then step lambda through the
spectrum and plot the tristimulus vector of each narrow band, you would
get this locus. Jozef preferred to use a different methodology and not
the orthonormal basis, but the result is the same. Therefore Cohen's
"locus of unit monochromats" is my "vectorial sensitivity to
wavelength." It was my privilege to know Jozef Cohen personally,
meeting
him several times and discussing his work with him and with Michael
Brill and others.
Note added 2004 November 4: As
of today, the page with URL monochromat.html
contains a new presentation entitled "Color Matching with Amplitude not
Left Out." The topic is still vectorial color, responding to the
question that arose in Ronald W. Everson's class in about 1974.
|