|
|||||||
|
|
||||
|
3 Main Ideas |
Orthonormal
Color Matching Functions.
|
Vectorial Sensitivity to narrow-band lights. |
Tristimulus vectors,
including amplitude. |
Early
Color-Matching Data of Guild or Wright. |
Thoughts on Matching Data |
The primary
lights are not unique, and the same facts can be
presented in alternate sets of graphs, an awkward
situation. However,
|
Same Color Matches, Varying Overlap among Functions: |
(x, y) Diagram |
So, we think in terms of the chromaticity diagram... | but
those primary colors are still needed. |
Which Wavelengths Act Most Strongly in Mixtures? |
MacAdam, and later Thornton did
calculations like this. Narrow-band lights of constant power are mixed with equal energy light. Some wavelengths perturb the white chromaticity more than others. Thornton coined the term "Prime Colors" for the 3 wavelengths that act most strongly. |
Color Matching
Functions Are in Fact Relatively Stable When the Primary Wavelengths Are Changed. |
|
Later Thornton found:
Michael
Brill's new theorem:
When
one primary wavelength is changed (say the red
wavelength only) then the associated (red) color
matching function changes only in scale, not in
shape.(For a more leisurely discussion of shifting primary wavelengths, please click here.) |
Prime Colors Must
Relate to the Overlap of Red and Green
Sensitivities... |
Red and green cone functions are
highly overlapping. |
Subtracting green from red gives
peaks to account for Prime Colors, but the scaling
is arbitrary. |
Achromatic sensitivity, y-bar,
is a linear combination of red and green. Find a
second combination that is orthogonal to it. |
One Degree of Freedom
Remains, to Re-mix ω1 and ω2, But Keep the Mixtures Orthogonal. |
|
We start with ω1
and ω2, which are linear combinations of
red and green cone functions, and are orthonormal.
Other orthonormal pairs of functions can be generated
by a rotation matrix: |
Now Make a
Parametric Plot of ω2 vs ω1. Bingo, the Shape is Invariant. |
Vectorial
Sensitivity to Wavelength. |
For each λ, the eye's
sensitivity is a vector, (ω2, ω1) . The spectrum locus is the eye's vectorial sensitivity to color. It is not a boundary. The spectrum locus is alternatively the vectorial color of narrow-band lights, at unit power. |
Prime colors are the wavelengths where radius is a local maximum! |
Stalking Prime
Colors in the 2 Dimensions of Red and
Green. |
Vectorial
Sensitivity to Wavelength, Now in 3 Dimensions. |
Features of the
Orthonormal System |
Applications |
Color Rendering by
Light Sources |
Oh, Yes,
Calculating Tristimulus Vectors. |
Let L be a light, that is a Spectral
Power Distribution.
The
calculation is essentially the same, but the
benefits of the orthonormal color matching
functions are tremendous!
|
Issues
Demystified: |
Special Credit |
William A. Thornton Michael H.
Brill (But
MacAdam gets a demerit for disparaging Cohen's
work.) Calculations
were done with O-matrix software. |
William A. Thornton
|
Jozef
B. Cohen
|
Background |
General Background,
including Thornton's and Cohen's work is in Render Asking: James A. Worthey, "Color rendering: asking the question," Color Research and Application 28(6):403-412, December 2003. Mathematical detail is in Render Calc: James A. Worthey, "Color rendering: a calculation that estimates colorimetric shifts," Color Research and Application 29(1):43-56, February 2004. See http://www.jimworthey.com
!
|
Seldom Asked
Questions (links) |
Stop |
Scroll No Farther |
Material Below Addresses Obscure Questions |
2 colors, C1 and C2
are set to equal power. The mixture is m. |
Cone Sensitivities,
Approximately Smith-Pokorny Primaries |
|
||
|
|
||
|
|
||
|
|
|
|
|
|||||||||||||
|
|
|
|
|
||
|
|